Implicit Cognition. Research on implicit
attitude formation and memory is one of the more active areas of inquiry
in cognitive and social psychology. Not surprisingly, numerous applications
have emerged in marketing, since consumer behavior is clearly the subject of
potential influence attempts (advertising, point-of-sale displays, etc.) that
are often beyond conscious control. New methods of measuring the strength
of implicit mental associations between constructs have recently emerged, led
by the Implicit Association Test. The Go-No-Go-Task is another such measure.
Memory Intrusions and
Reconstruction of Knowledge. Psychological research into cognitive processes has
shown that memory is constructive and its retrieval mechanisms are often times
imperfect, commonly suffering from biases, distortions, and intrusions. The
associative view of memory as a network of connected nodes includes the concept
of retrieval paths, the routes allowing for the recovery of previously acquired
knowledge from long-term storage. However, the very same connections can also
serve as sources of memory errors, as the interweaving of new information and
prior knowledge has the apparent potential to create confusion and recall
mistakes.
Social Identification Salience and In-Group
Bias. The fundamental basis of
group-based processing of advertising content is self-other categorization.
People tend to classify others on the basis of their similarities or
dissimilarities with the evaluator and this processing creates an in-group
(individuals perceived as members of the same category as self) and an
out-group (members of a different category). The practice of target
marketing (addressing distinct segments of consumers) is one instance where
issues of group bias become very important.
Believability and Persuasion. Research on consumer decision making looks at
how consumers make their choices by presenting them with product information
and observing how they use it to evaluate one or more products. However, in
everyday decision making, consumers may have been exposed to and possess
product information that is not true.Truthful sources of unfavorable product
information would be product reviews presented in the media and word of mouth
from dissatisfied customers. False or untruthful product information often
serves as the basis for disparaging rumors.
Self-Reference and Persuasion. Most of the literature on persuasion has
embraced the intuitive assumption that self-referencing works via the direct
activation of memory chunks relevant to the topic at hand. The subsequent
formation of affective linkages between the message object (e.g. advertised
brand) and the self translates into a powerful illusion of cognitive response
ownership that is actually message-originated and induced. A recent meta-analysis
of self-referencing research confirms this mnemonic superiority, but is less
definitive in terms of explaining why the self-referencing effect occurs.
Meaning
Access in Polysemy. Linguistics and cognition research
have established that the study of expressions with multiple meanings must
adequately account for two critical elements that directly affect both
comprehensions and recall. The first one is the base frequency of each meaning,
and the other aspect refers to the contextual information present at the time
of exposure. Most research on literal polysemy
resides within the perception and cognition literature, with few consumer
applications on record.
Measurement
Issues: Judgment Biases in Brand Attitudes. The
schematic model of a "shifting standards" effect in judgments argues
that commonly used stereotypes are in some cases likely to bias evaluations by
changing the meaning of the scale across stimuli. When stable, externally
anchored units of measurement are used, objective rating reflect subjects' true
mental representations and are likely to confirm prevalent stereotypes.
However, when subjective scales are employed, judges differentially adjust the
meanings of the anchors, thus hiding prevalent stereotypes.
Measuring
Brand Globality. Data suggest that American consumers
have little explicit notion of what characterizes a global brand beyond its
wide recognition and availability across markets. In particular, the
association with higher quality is not as strong as previous research has
suggested, and previous research on globality effects appears to have
confounded globality and brand strength. However, American consumers are
generally unable to suppress their largely favorable attitudes towards global
brands in automatic response tests, even if their explicit attitude towards
global brands is largely negative.
Consumer Response to Modular Products. Studies show that
consumers tend to discount the cost savings
associated with modular upgrades excessively (insufficiently) when the time
between the initial purchase and the upgrade is short (long). Thus modular
upgradability as a product feature has higher profit potential for slowly
rather than rapidly improving ones. There is also a preference reversal between
the initial purchase (when people foresee making a full product replacement in
the future and the eventual point of upgrade (when they are more likely to revert to modular
upgrades).
Brand Equity Effects on Stock Performance. The EquiTrend brand
equity measure assesses consumer allegiance
to a brand, and is thus largely exogenous to the stock market. The
Interbrand brand values, by contrast, rely partly on financial
projections which are necessarily predicated on specific assumptions about
future growth. If the assumptions used to generate Interbrand’s projected
brand values no longer hold (as is the case in times of economic crises), the
calculated brand values will be misleading. By contrast, the EquiTrend
measure is largely unaffected and a significant predictor of stock performance
during crises (beyond the Fama-French factors).